Skip to main content

Brainpower Wins Over Brawn When Male “Hermit” Hummingbirds Display for Mates

By Hugh Powell
Long-billed Hermit by Chris Jimenez
Long-billed Hermit in Braulio Carrillo National Park, Costa Rica. Photo by Chris Jiménez.

From the Summer 2018 issue of Living Bird magazine. Subscribe now.

When male animals compete over mates, it’s often a showy affair: think of elk tangling antlers or tom turkeys strutting. But for a Costa Rican hummingbird, it seems mental prowess holds the edge over mere physical flamboyance.

New experiments show that dominant male Long-billed Hermits have better spatial memories and sing more consistent songs than less successful males, according to research published in the journal Scientific Reports. The benefit of a good spatial memory even outweighs the advantages of bigger body size and extra flight power.

“When we think about smart animals, we usually think about dolphins, crows, primates,” says Marcelo Araya-Salas, a Cornell Lab of Ornithology postdoctoral fellow and lead author of the paper. “But it’s everywhere. Every animal is going to gain some clear advantage from learning about their environment. As behavioral ecologists, we are starting to unveil this other side of animal behavior.”

The Long-billed Hermit is common in the rainforests of Costa Rica. It’s about twice the size of the familiar Ruby-throated Hummingbird, with a long, curved bill just right for sipping nectar from brilliant-orange heliconia flowers. (View our Long-billed Hermit account in Birds of the World.)

Long-billed Hermit by Chris Jiminez
Studies like this one prompt a reconsideration of what it means to be "intelligent." Photo of a Long-billed Hermit by Chris Jiménez.

Males perch in the forest understory and sing incessantly, every day during their 8-month breeding season, at display sites known as leks. The dominant males fight over coveted singing perches, sometimes stabbing at each other with specially sharpened bill tips. Less dominant males, known as “floaters,” come and go from the lek but aren’t able to defend a singing perch.

Displaying males risk losing their spot each time they leave to refuel, so there’s a premium on getting to nectar-rich flowers quickly. But feeding trips are like a giant game of concentration, with each bird often flying for a mile and having to choose among thousands of blooms to get their fill.

Araya-Salas and his colleagues devised a simple experiment to test how well males could remember reliable food sources. They set up a row of three hummingbird feeders near the leks. Two contained only water; one had sugar water. The researchers watched the birds make a first trip and discover which feeder held the goods. Then they waited until the birds came back and recorded whether they found the sugar water on their first try. In all, they tested 30 male hermits from 3 leks, with most of the males completing at least 10 trials.

“There was one male who got all the trials right; he never missed the rewarding feeder,” Araya-Salas says. “And there was a male that did no better than you would expect by chance, which is a third of the time. So there was the whole range of performance on display.”

Some key aspects of Long-billed Hermit behavior and the experimental setup are illustrated in this video. Courtesy Marcelo Araya-Salas.

Birds that showed better spatial memories were consistently more likely to be dominant birds with perches at the lek, while the floaters were the ones that couldn’t remember which feeder was which. The pattern was independent of age, so it wasn’t just that older males had both better memories and more dominant positions. Even more surprising, better memories won out over physique: birds with bigger bodies, or a larger, sharper bill point, or that could fly with more power (as measured by their ability to briefly lift a small weight), still couldn’t win space at the lek as well as the males with good memories.

Araya-Salas says the spatial memory could help in two ways—by making foraging trips faster, or by helping the males keep track of where their rivals sat within the lek, making it easier to defend against them.

The researchers also noted that males with better spatial memory also sang more consistent songs. It’s thought this ability is attractive to females, because it means the singer sounds less like an inexperienced youngster and more like a veteran survivor.

It remains to be seen how well the spatial memory task translates to the much more complicated task of finding rewarding flowers in the real-life rainforest, as well as what specific mental tasks the dominant hummingbirds are excelling at. Araya-Salas says it’s hard to compare Long-billed Hermits against the bird world’s most famous memory artists, birds like the Pinyon Jay, Clark’s Nutcracker, and Gray Jay, because the tasks—remembering flower locations from day to day vs. stashing seeds in the ground for months—are so different. But the exciting thing, he says, is the recognition that cognitive abilities are so important.

“We always think we are the smart species—we are the ones with game-changing intelligence.” Araya-Salas says. “But it’s a matter of degree, and we are more similar to the other animals than we think.”

The research was supported by the National Geographic Society, New Mexico State University, the Organization for Tropical Studies, and the British Ornithologists’ Union. In addition to Araya-Salas, the research team consisted of Patricia Gonzalez-Gomez of the University of California, Davis; Katarzyna Wojczulanis-Jakubas of the University of Gdansk, Poland; Virgilio López III of the University of Connecticut; and Timothy F. Wright of New Mexico State University.

 

All About Birds
is a free resource

Available for everyone,
funded by donors like you

American Kestrel by Blair Dudeck / Macaulay Library